Waiting
Login processing...

Trial ends in Request Full Access Tell Your Colleague About Jove

Behavior

Detecting Behavioral Deficits in Rats After Traumatic Brain Injury

Published: January 30, 2018 doi: 10.3791/56044
* These authors contributed equally

Summary

The goal of the behavioral tests presented here is to detect functional deficits in rats after traumatic brain injury. Four specific tests are presented that detect deficits in behaviors to reflect the damage to specific brain areas at times extending to one year after injury.

Abstract

With the increasing incidence of traumatic brain injury (TBI) in both civilian and military populations, TBI is now considered a chronic disease; however, few studies have investigated the long-term effects of injury in rodent models of TBI. Shown here are behavioral measures that are well-established in TBI research for times early after injury, such as two weeks, until two months. Some of these methods have previously been used at later times after injury, up to one year, but by very few laboratories. The methods demonstrated here are a short neurological assessment to test reflexes, a Beam-Balance to test balance, a Beam-Walk to test balance and motor coordination, and a working memory version of the Morris water maze that can be sensitive to deficits in reference memory. Male rats were handled and pre-trained to neurological, balance, and motor coordination tests prior to receiving parasagittal fluid percussion injury (FPI) or sham injury. Rats can be tested on the short neurological assessment (neuroscore), the beam-balance, and the Beam-Walk multiple times, while testing on the water maze can only be done once. This difference is because rats can remember the task, thus confounding the results if repeated testing is attempted in the same animal. When testing from one to three days after injury, significant differences are detected in all three non-cognitive tasks. However, differences in the Beam-Walk task were not detectable at later time points (after 3 months). Deficits were detected at 3 months in the Beam-Balance and at 6 months in the neuroscore. Deficits in working memory were detected out to 12 months after injury, and a deficit in a reference memory first appeared at 12 months. Thus, standard behavioral tests can be useful measures of persistent behavioral deficits after FPI.

Introduction

The methods presented here are designed to detect functional deficits in specific brain areas induced by an experimental model of TBI in the rat. Four different behavior tests will be described. First, the short neurological assessment, referred to as the neuroscore, can be performed without requiring any specialized equipment but does require practice; this test detects deficits in reflexes. Second, the Beam-Balance test detects deficits in the ability to balance. This task requires the handler to score the rat based on an ordinal scale and requires some training of the handler. The Beam-Balance test requires a narrow beam and is sensitive to deficits in the vestibular system. The third test assesses vestibulomotor coordination. This test is known as the Beam-Walk task, and although some pre-training of the rat is required, this procedure is more objective than the previous two as the latency to traverse the beam is an objective measure not dependent on subjective scoring. This difference is because the time to traverse a narrow beam to reach a safe box is measured. The Beam-Walk test requires a longer beam than the Beam-Balance as well as an escape box. This test measures deficits in both motor coordination and balance and thus is sensitive to damage to the cerebellum and motor related brain areas. The working memory version of the Morris water maze (MWM-WM) primarily tests hippocampal function and integration with the prefrontal cortex or executive function. The version of the Morris water maze shown can also be used to detect deficits in reference memory1.

These methods were chosen based on their well-established track record in the literature. Each one has been effective in many hands from different laboratories with multiple strains of rats over numerous years of research. However, in the past, post-injury measures up to two weeks after injury were considered "chronic" time points. Thus, to establish behavioral techniques for the study of chronic effects of TBI in rodents, these well-known methods needed to be evaluated for sensitivity to detect TBI induced deficits at longer time points after injury. While there are now several rodent models of TBI, the FPI model is one of the most widely used, and is applied in this study. This model was first published in the 1950's2, and since then, more than 1,000 papers have employed FPI in rats3. The neuropathology of this type of injury has been well-described by us4 and others5,6,7. Briefly, neuronal injury in the hippocampus has been shown to be dose-dependent using Fluoro-Jade staining at short times after injury, i.e., 24-48 h; while gross atrophy and cavitation including thinning of the internal capsule and cortex has been reported at one year after injury6,7.

The most meaningful representation of brain function is assessed by using behavioral outcome measures after an experimental brain injury. However, the vast majority of FPI experiments that use behavioral outcomes make measures relatively early, typically from 1 to 14 days after injury. Using the methods demonstrated here, some behavioral deficits can be detected out to 12 months after injury1.Neurological function, gross vestibulomotor function, and fine motor coordination were assessed on post-injury days (PIDs) 1-3 and at 3, 6, and 12 months after surgery, using a short neurological assessment (Neuroscore; modified from Schallert8), the Beam-Balance task, and the Beam-Walk task9,10,11. Reference and working memory were assessed using a working memory version of the Morris water maze1,12,13.

Protocol

All animal experiments are first approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Texas Medical Branch, Galveston, Texas as directed by the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (8th Edition, National Research Council).

1. Surgical Procedures and Fluid Percussion TBI

  1. Obtain adult male 300 g Sprague-Dawley rats from a vendor and house two per cage with food and water ad libitum in a vivarium with constant conditions: light cycle (600 h to 1,800 h), temperature (21 °C to 23 °C), and humidity (40-60%).
  2. Prior to surgery, handle the rats for three to five days, and then train the rats for the Neuroscore, Beam-Balance, and Beam-Walk procedures from one to three days prior to baseline assessment. Conduct the baseline assessment either the day or morning prior to the surgery.
    NOTE: Always prepare control rats, such as sham-operated or surgically naïve rats in the same manner as those that will receive an injury and, either randomly or in a balanced manner, group the rats into the treatment groups.
  3. Perform surgery under aseptic conditions (sterile instruments, clean surgical gowns, sterile gloves, masks and head covers).
  4. Anesthetize rats using isoflurane at 4% for induction and 1.5-2% for maintenance. Intubate and mechanically ventilate the rats (using isoflurane in air:oxygen (70:30) and prepare for parasagittal fluid-percussion injury as previously described14,15.
  5. Infiltrate wound sites with 0.10% bupivicane prior to suturing and insert rectal acetaminophen suppository (120 mg/kg) prior to awakening from anesthesia. Monitor rats for at least 4 h during the recovery period and the following day for signs of infection, severe neurological injury (e.g. paralysis) or severe discomfort (e.g. persistent dormouse position).
    NOTE: Rats exhibiting any of these symptoms should be euthanized (4% isoflurane in an anesthetic chamber followed by decapitation).

2. Neuroscore Training and Testing

  1. Neuroscore training
    1. For training on rats known to be experimentally naïve, run through the tests in the following order (steps 2.2.1-2.2.5) from start to finish, return to home cage for 1 min, then repeat until a score of zero is achieved.
    2. Mark scores on the score sheet for a record of the training trials for each rat. After training, perform one test session (see below) on the same or following day.
      NOTE: If the test session does not produce a zero score for the baseline, training and testing may be repeated or the rat may be diverted to a non-behavior experiment.
  2. Neuroscore testing
    NOTE: Run through the tests in the following order, return to home cage for 1 min, then repeat twice for a total of three times.
    1. Forelimb flexion test
      1. Lift the rat by the tail and hold about 6 - 12 inches above the table surface.
      2. Observe whether the rat extends or flexes forelimbs. Score the presence of flexion (1) or absence (0).
        NOTE: Flexion is abnormal. Possible score of 1 x 3 = 3 (Total possible = 3).
    2. Hindlimb flexion test
      1. Lift the rat by the tail and hold about 6 - 12 inches above the table surface.
      2. Observe whether the rat extends or flexes hindlimbs. Score the presence of flexion (1) or absence (0).
        NOTE: Flexion is abnormal. Possible score of 1 x 3 = 3 (Cumulative Total possible = 6).
    3. Visually triggered placing test
      1. Lift the rat by the tail.
      2. Slowly lower the rat toward the edge of the table until the nose is about 10 cm from the edge.
      3. Move the rat slowly toward the edge (do not allow the whiskers to touch the edge).
      4. Observe whether the rat reaches and extends forepaws towards the table. Score the presence (0) or absence (1) of extending forepaws.
        NOTE: Reaching for the table in response to visual cues is normal. Possible score of 1 x 3 = 3 (Cumulative Total possible = 9).
    4. Contact triggered placing test
      1. Hold the rat, with body in hand, parallel to table edge and forelegs free.
      2. Slowly lower the rat toward the edge of table until the whiskers on one side touch the edge of the table.
      3. Observe whether the rat extends the forelimb on the same side as the whiskers that are touching the table toward the table edge as soon as the whiskers touch.
        NOTE: Elicitation of this response takes considerable practice and researchers need to be well-trained to perform this test consistently.
      4. Score the presence (0) or absence (1) of reaching toward the table.
        NOTE: Reaching in response to tactile stimulation is normal. Possible score of 1 x 3 = 3 (Total possible = 12).
      5. Repeat steps 2.2.4.1-2.2.4.4 for the opposite side. Possible score of 1 x 3 = 3 (Cumulative Total possible = 15).
    5. Hindpaw grasping reflex test
      1. Hold the rat in the one hand with thumb and index finger around the chest under the forelimbs.
      2. Gently touch the palm of the one hindpaw with the other forefinger.
      3. Observe whether the rat grasps the forefinger. Score the presence (0) or absence (1) of grasping.
        NOTE: Grasping is normal. Possible score of 1 x 3 = 3 (Total possible = 18).
      4. Repeat steps 2.2.5.1-2.2.5.3 for the opposite side. Possible score of 1 x 3 = 3 (Total possible = 21).
    6. Scoring
      1. Sum the scores for a possible cumulative total of 7 x 3 = 21. A score of zero is normal.

3. Beam-Balance Training and Testing

  1. Equipment
    1. Use a beam 60 cm in length, 1.75 cm in width, 4.0 cm in height, set 90 cm off the floor, with a barrier 30 cm in height, 30 cm in width. Secure the beam to a table with the barrier attached so that 50 cm of the beam protrudes from the barrier, away from the table.
    2. Place a cushioned safety box under the beam to soften the impact of rats that fall.
  2. Beam-Balance training
    1. At 24-48 h before surgery, place the rat on the beam for a 60 s trial.
    2. If the rat fails to balance on its own, allow the rat to fall into the safety box.
    3. Begin timing when the rat is securely positioned on the beam.
    4. Observe the rat for the 60 s period and rate its performance based on the following scale: 1 = Shows stable balance (grooms, walks, attempts to climb the barrier), 2 = Shows shaky balance (grasps sides of beam and/or has unsteady movements), 3 = Tries to balance but slips or spins on the beam, hangs on by hugging the beam, 4 = Tries to balance, but falls after 10 s, 5 = Hangs over or from the beam and falls off in under 10 s, 6 = Falls off, making no effort to balance or hang onto the beam.
    5. Record the score on the worksheet.
    6. Allow the rat to rest for 15 s in home cage, then repeat steps 3.2.1-3.2.5 until the rat achieves three scores of 1 or 2. The rat is then considered trained.
    7. Perform one pre-assessment at 24 h or on the day of surgery prior to surgery.
  3. Beam-Balance testing
    1. Starting 24 h after surgery and continuing for up to 4 days, test the rats daily.
      1. Place the rat on the beam and start the timer. Observe the rat closely for 60 s. Record the score on the worksheet.
      2. Return the rat to the home cage for a brief rest period (1-3 min).
      3. Repeat steps 3.3.1.1-3.3.1.2 for a total of three tests.

4. Beam-Walk Training and Testing

  1. Equipment
    1. Use a wooden beam 100 cm in length, 2.5 cm in width, and 4.0 cm in height.
    2. Prepare an adjustable stand, an adjustable table, and four pegs, 2 cm in height, and a black goal box 28 cm in length, 18 cm in height, and 18 cm in width, with an opening on one end large enough for the rat to pass through.
    3. Attach the target end of the beam to the open side of the goal box that is placed on the adjustable table. Place the bright light and white noise generator near the starting end of the beam. The starting end of the beam is fixed to the adjustable stand so the beam and box are at the same level, about 1 m above the floor.
  2. Beam-Walk training
    1. Start training 24-48 h before surgery.
    2. Place the rat in the goal box for 1 min. After 1 min, remove the rat and start the trial.
    3. To start the trial, turn on the light and white noise and place the rat on the beam at the location of the peg hole closest to the goal box and allow the rat to enter the goal box.
    4. When the rat's front feet cross the threshold of the goal box, immediately turn off the light and noise sources (this is the end of one trial).
    5. Allow the rat to rest in the goal box for 30 s between each trial.
    6. Repeat the procedure in step 4.2.3-4.2.5 twice at each peg location and from the starting position. Insert the pegs in the holes and run one complete Beam-Walk with the pegs in place.
    7. Run three timed Beam-Walk trials.
      1. Remove the rat from the goal box. Turn on the light and white noise and start the stopwatch when placing the rat on the beam. Stop the stop watch immediately when the rat's front feet cross the threshold of the goal box, and then immediately turn off the light and noise.
      2. Record the time on the worksheet.
      3. Repeat steps 4.2.7.1-4.2.7.2 until the rat has achieved three times of 5 s or less. The rat is now considered trained.
  3. Beam-Walk baseline assessment
    1. On the day or morning prior to surgery, do three timed trials with pegs in place.
    2. Start by placing the rat in the goal box for 30 s. Remove the rat from the goal box and turn on the white noise and light. Place the rat on the start end of the beam and simultaneously start the stop watch. When the rat's front feet cross the threshold of the goal box, immediately turn off the light and noise sources and stop the timer.
    3. Record the time on the worksheet. Allow the rat to remain in the goal box for 30 s.
    4. Repeat steps 4.3.2-4.3.3 until three latencies are recorded on the worksheet. Return the rat to the home cage after three timed trials are completed.
  4. Beam-Walk testing
    1. Test the rats daily starting 24 h after surgery and continuing for up to 4 days. Perform three timed trials as in steps 4.3.

5. Working Memory Water Maze

  1. Equipment
    1. Use a tank filled with water to a height of 28 cm and maintained at 26 ± 1 °C.
    2. Use a clear acrylic glass platform that is 10 cm in diameter on a stand 26 cm in height.
      NOTE: The top surface of the platform should be coated with silicon in the shape of a circle with an X across it. This allows the rats to climb up onto the platform and gives them traction so they do not slip off.
    3. Gather a stopwatch, a heat lamp, disposable towels, absorbent pads, extra cages, and a small, long-handled aquarium fish net. Use a computerized video tracking system that is connected to a video camera to record the rat swimming and send the data to the computer. Save the video and data on the computer for later analysis.
  2. Working memory water maze testing
    1. Give the rats four pairs of trials each day for five consecutive days, place the platform in each of the four quadrants and start the rats from each of the four starting points (N, S, E, W) as described below.
    2. First define the starting location-platform pairs to be used throughout the experiment.
      NOTE: The order of the quadrants where the platform is located and the starting point used needs to be in a different order for each of the five days of swimming, but the same for each rat.
      1. Use four starting points (N, S, E, or W) and four platform locations (Quadrants 1, 2, 3, or 4; Figure 1). For example, (N, 2; E, 4; S, 1; W, 3; see Figure 1). Plan a balanced order (not random) to avoid starting points too close to the platforms (No starting point is the same quadrant as the platform location). Set up a data sheet using the platform quadrants and four starting points.
      2. Write a protocol for the video tracking software to use in order to video the rats swimming and to collect specified data (e.g., duration of swim, speed, distance traveled before finding the platform).
        NOTE: The tracking software will automatically stop recording after the specified duration. The protocol should allow for specifying where the platform is placed, how many trials to run per animal, and how many animals will be tested per session, and also the maximum duration allowed (e.g., 120 s).
      3. Test 4-6 rats per session.
        NOTE: More than 6 rats create an issue in the timing between rats and can lead to error by the handler. The warming boxes also become crowded.
    3. Trial 1
      1. Open the video tracking software and load the correct protocol including the water maze map.
      2. Place the platform in the assigned location (e.g., 2; Figure 1) and check that it matches the map in the software. Prepare the tracking software to start when the rat enters the field of view of the video camera.
      3. Place the rat in the tank facing the wall at the assigned location (e.g., N; Figure 1) and immediately start the timer.
      4. Allow the rat 120 s to find the platform. When the rat finds the platform, stop the timer and record the time on the worksheet. If the rat fails to find the platform, lead it to the platform by hand and record 120 s. Allow the rat 15 s to remain on the platform.
    4. Trial 2
      1. Check that the software is ready for Trial 2. Place the rat back in the tank at the same starting position (N). Repeat step 5.2.3.4.
    5. After Trial 2, place the rat in the heated enclosure for 4 min. Move the platform to the second location (4; Figure 1), and check that it matches the map in the software.
    6. Repeat Trial 1 and 2 procedures (steps 5.2.3-5.2.4) until all four starting location/platform pairings are completed.

6. Data Analysis

  1. Neuroscore
    1. Manually transfer the handwritten results to a computer spreadsheet.
    2. Sum the results for each trial to obtain three scores per rat on each day.
    3. Format the data for statistical analysis (either in the long or wide format depending on the software preference).
      NOTE: The long format has a single column for treatment (in this case, populated with either "Naïve", "Sham", or "TBI"), a single column for day (in this case "0", "1", "2" or "3"), and a single column for trial (either "1", "2" or "3"). The wide format has a single column for each combination of factor levels (so a single column for Naïve, day 0, trial 1, another column for Naïve, day 0, trial 2, etc.)
    4. Average the score for each animal on each day. Since there were three trials done on each day there will be three values for each animal per day.
    5. Assess whether the data are normally distributed. Use a nonparametric statistical (e.g., the Kruskal-Wallis) test to analyze whether the score on each day is different between groups. In this case, since these data are not continuous, they are not normally distributed.
    6. To determine where the differences lie, do post-hoc tests, such as the Tukey's post-hoc analysis.
      NOTE: Here, the R statistical software package16, the Kruskal.test() function and the posthoc.kruskal.nemenyi.test function within the Pairwise Multiple Comparison of Mean Ranks Package (PMCMR)17 were used.
    7. Additionally, test to see if there are any differences between days within each group.
      NOTE: For example, to see if the SHAM animals behave differently on Day 0 compared to Day 1, Day 2, or Day 3. To do this, run a one-way repeated measures ANOVA. This can be accomplished in R using the ezANOVA function within the ez package.
    8. To run a repeated measures ANOVA, first check the assumption about sphericity.
      NOTE: Here, the data indicate that the within factor (Day) does meet the sphericity assumption for NAÏVE and SHAM, but not for TBI. Thus, a correction is not necessary for NAÏVE or SHAM. For the TBI data, use the Greenhouse-Geisser correction.
    9. If significant differences are found, run a post-hoc test to determine where the differences lie. This is achieved in R using the pairwise t-test function. Plot the results as a box plot, as shown in the representative results (Figure 2).
  2. Beam-Balance
    1. Manually transfer the handwritten scores to a computer spreadsheet. Format the data for statistical analysis (either in long or wide format depending on the software preference). See Note in step 6.1.3.
    2. Average the scores for each rat on each day so that each rat will have one score per day. To test whether the score on each day is different between NAÏVE, SHAM, and TBI, assess whether the data are normally distributed.
      NOTE: In this case, since the data are not continuous, these data are not normally distributed. Therefore, use a nonparametric statistical test (e.g., the Kruskal-Wallis test).
    3. To determine where the differences lie, do post-hoc tests, e.g., Tukey's post-hoc analysis. To test for differences between days within each treatment group, run a one-way repeated measures ANOVA (see step 6.1.7). Check the assumption about sphericity.
      NOTE: In this study, the data indicate that the within factor (Day) does not meet the sphericity assumption for any of the groups, so used continuity corrections. Use the Greenhouse-Grier continuity correction.
    4. Plot the results on a box plot as shown in the representative results (Figure 3).
  3. Beam-walk
    1. Manually transfer handwritten results to a computer spreadsheet. Average the three Beam-Walk latencies for each animal for each day. Format the data for statistical analysis (see step 6.1.3).
    2. Assess whether the data is normally distributed.
      NOTE: In this case, the data is continuous, and is normally distributed. Therefore, use a one-way ANOVA to determine if the latency on each day is different between Naïve, SHAM, and TBI.
    3. To see if there is any difference between days within a treatment group, run a one-way repeated measures ANOVA. First check the assumption about sphericity.
      NOTE: In this study, the data indicate that the within factor (Day) does not meet the sphericity assumption for any of our groups, so continuity corrections are used. Use the Greenhouse-Grier correction.
    4. Plot the results on a box plot as shown in the representative results (Figure 4).
  4. Working memory water maze
    1. Transfer the data from the worksheet or computer tracking program to a spreadsheet. Select the outcomes to be analyzed.
      NOTE: Many possible outcomes are available for analysis from computer tracking programs. Examples of outcomes selected for analysis may include: latency, path length, thigmotaxia, and swim speed. The most commonly reported outcome is latency, as used in the provided example.
    2. Format the data for statistical analysis (either in long or wide format depending on the software preference).
      NOTE: The long format has a single column for treatment (in this case, populated with either "Naïve", "Sham", or "TBI"), a single column for day (in this case "1", "2", "3", "4", or "5") and a single column for trial (either "1", "2", "3", "4", "5", "6", "7", or "8"). We also need an additional column to identify the attempt (either "1" or "2"). Wide format has a single column for each combination of factor levels (so for example, a single column for Naïve, day 1, trial 1, attempt 1, another column for Naïve, day 1, trial 2, attempt 2). Also, the difference between Trial 1 and Trial 2 can be calculated for each session and analyzed as a difference score.
    3. To find if there is an overall difference between the injury groups, perform the following steps.
      1. First, average the water maze latency for each animal for Day 1.
        NOTE: There were four sessions on each day, so average the four values per animal for each of Trial 1 and Trial 2. Do this calculation for the remaining days as well.
      2. To check for injury differences overall, run a two-way repeated measures ANOVA. There are two factors, injury and day. Injury is a between group factor and day is a within group factor. Note: Here R was used.
      3. If the results indicate a significant difference due to Injury, then run a Tukey's post-hoc test to see where the differences lie.
    4. To find out whether there are differences between the injury groups on specific days perform the following steps.
      NOTE: Day 1 is used as an example, and the same analysis needs to be done for all the following days. Also, this analysis is done in several ways, first for Trial 1 only, second for Trial 2 only, and third for the difference between Trial 1 and Trial 2. Trial 1 is used as an example; the same steps need to be used for the other analyses.
      1. First, average the water maze latency for each animal for Day 1. Since there were four repetitions of "Trial 1" on each day, average the four values for each animal.
      2. Assess whether the data is normally distributed.
        NOTE: In this case, the data is continuous, and is normally distributed. Therefore, use the one-way ANOVA to determine if the water maze latency on Day 1 is different between NAÏVE, SHAM, and TBI. R statistical software package and the aov() function were used here.
      3. Use a 5% level of significance. If the resulting p-value is less than 0.05, then there are significant differences between the groups.
      4. To determine where those differences lie, use the Tukey's post-hoc test. This is the TukeyHSD() function in R.
    5. To find out if there are differences between days within treatment groups, follow these steps.
      1. First, run a one-way repeated measures ANOVA. This can be accomplished in R using the ezANOVA function within the ez package.
      2. Before running a repeated measures ANOVA, first check the assumption about sphericity.
        NOTE: The within factor (Day) meets the sphericity assumption for all of the groups, thus there is no need to use continuity corrections.
      3. If differences between days are found (p-values less than 0.05), then run a post-hoc test to determine exactly where the differences lie. This step is achieved in R using the pairwise.t.test function.
    6. Graph the results using line graphs (Figure 5). Also, Trial 1-Trial 2 can be graphed.

Representative Results

Results of the neuroscore procedure (Figure 2) demonstrate both the potential for false positive (SHAM and TBI groups at day 0) and the sensitivity of this test to detect small differences. False positives can occur when the rat is not well habituated to the procedure, so it is not fully relaxed. Day 0 is prior to surgery, so ideally all rats should reach the criterion of a score of 0 prior to entering a study. Days 1-3 demonstrate the sensitivity of this test to detect small changes in the score. While there is a potential for a score as high as 21, scores higher than 3 are unusual in this model. In this example, repeated measures ANOVA revealed no differences between days for NAÏVE (p = 0.78) or SHAM (p = 0.09); however, for the TBI group there were differences between days (p < 0.05). Post-hoc pairwise comparison indicated that Day 0 is significantly different from Days 1, 2, and 3. This result demonstrates that the injury produced small yet significant changes in the neurological assessment.

Further analysis using the Kruskal-Wallis test compared NAÏVE, SHAM, and TBI on each day, followed by the Tukey's post-hoc test to determine exactly where the differences lie. For Day 0, the test statistic was 13.37, p = 0.001, and SHAM was significantly different from NAÏVE (p = 0.008). Ideally, there should be no differences between groups on Day 0, as no treatments or procedures have been administered. In this case, the rats should be further habituated to the procedure, or transferred to a non-behavior study. For Day 1, the test statistic was 32.39, p = 9.75e-8, with the post-hoc test, indicating that SHAM and TBI were significantly different from NAÏVE (p = 0.002, p = 5.9e-7, respectively). For Day 2, the test statistic was 23.39, p = 8.34e-6, and SHAM and TBI were different from NAÏVE (p = 0.002, p = 6.8e-5). For Day 3, the test statistic was 38.4, p = 4.59e-9, and again, SHAM and TBI were significantly different from NAÏVE (p = 0.001, p = 2.1e-8, respectively). These results point to the fact that the SHAM preparation also produces some deficits in neurological assessment at times early after injury.

Representative Beam-Balance results (Figure 3) demonstrate the sensitivity of the Beam-Balance test to deficits shortly after injury (Figure 3, left) and at a time point longer after injury (Figure 3, right). The sensitivity of the Beam-Balance test to the effects of brain injury diminishes over time, because as the uninjured rats age and gain weight, they have increased difficulty balancing on the beam. At later time points, the beam is turned so the rats are balancing on the wider side of the beam. Nevertheless, by 6 months after injury, this test is no longer sensitive to the effects of injury as age and/or weight confound the ability to perform the task (Figure 3, right). Alternatively, healing may have occurred in the vestibular system, and these data accurately reflect that the rats' ability to balance reaches the same level as the control groups.

In comparing Naïve, SHAM, and TBI on each day, we used the Kruskal-Wallis test. The results for time points early after injury are shown in Figure 3, left. On Day 0, the Kruskal-Wallis test found the value of the test statistic to be 6.81, p = 0.033. There was a significant difference between the groups, with the Tukey's post-hoc test showing that the Naïve group was different than SHAM (p = 0.038); however, all three groups had means well below 2.0, indicating that all rats had met the criteria to continue. It would be preferable to have no differences between groups on Day 0, but since all groups are below 2, they can continue in the study. On PID 1, the Kruskal-Wallis test statistic was 69.72, p = 7.25e-16. The Tukey's post-hoc test showed that the TBI group was significantly different from both the Naïve and Sham groups (p = 4.9e-14, p = 9.1e-08, respectively). On Day 2, the Kruskal-Wallis test statistic was 62.84 and p = 2.26e-14, with the post-hoc test showing TBI different from NAÏVE and SHAM (p = 1.0e-10, p = 2.1e-10 respectively). On Day 3, the Kruskal-Wallis test statistic was 62.69 and p = 2.44e-14. The post-hoc test showed TBI different from Naïve and SHAM, (p = 9.6e-12, p = 1.7e-08, respectively). We additionally looked to see if there were any differences between days within each group. Using a repeated measures ANOVA, for NAÏVE, there were no differences between days (p = 0.367). For SHAM and TBI there were differences between days (p = 0.002, p = 3.90e-29, respectively). Post-hoc pairwise comparisons revealed for SHAM Day 1 is significantly different from Day 2 and Day 3 (p = 0.001, p = 0.01, respectively), and for TBI, Day 0 is significantly different form Days 1, 2, and 3 (p < 2e-16, p = 5.5e-16, and p = 2.7e-13, respectively). Day 1 is also significantly different from Day 3 (p = 0.036).

At 6 months after injury, comparisons between NAÏVE, SHAM, and TBI were made on each day using the Kruskal-Wallis test (Figure 3, right). On Day 0, the value of the test statistic was 3.36 and p = 0.187, so there were no differences on Day 0. All means were below 2, indicating that all rats and groups met the criteria to continue in the study. On PID 1, the test statistic was 6.11, p = 0.047; however, post-hoc analysis using Tukey's post-hoc test showed that none of the groups were significantly different when accounting for multiple hypothesis testing. On Day 2, the test statistic was 4.09, p = 0.13, ns, and on Day 3, the test statistic was 2.91, p = 0.23, ns. Thus, there were no differences between the injury groups on any given day.

Additionally, looking at differences between days within treatment groups, a repeated measures ANOVA revealed significant differences between days for NAÏVE, SHAM, and TBI (p = 0.0003, p = 2.61e-5, p = 5.59e-7, respectively; Figure 3, right). Post-hoc tests demonstrated the following differences. For NAÏVE, Day 0 was significantly different from Days 1, 2, and 3 (p = 0.002, p = 0.044, p = 0.004, respectively). For SHAM, all days were significantly different from each other: Day 0 was significantly different from Days 1, 2, and 3 (p = 0.0006, p = 0.001, p = 0.0006, respectively); Day 1 was significantly different from Days 2 and 3 (p = 0.031, p = 0.0006, respectively); and Day 2 was significantly different from Day 3 (p = 0.044). For TBI, Day 0 is significantly different from Days 1, 2, and 3 (p = 0.0005, p = 0.0008, p = 0.0005, respectively).

The results of the Beam-Walk test are shown at two time points (Figure 4). Similar to the Beam-Balance, this test detects deficits early after injury (Figure 4, left). However, by 6 months after injury, there are no significant differences between the groups (Figure 4, right), suggesting healing occurred in the injured group. This result may reflect effects of more advanced age and increased weight.

To compare NAÏVE, SHAM, and TBI on each day early after injury, a one-way ANOVA was used. There were no differences on Day 0 (F = 0.859, p = 0.426) and all latencies were below 5 s, indicating that all rats met the criteria to continue in the study. On PID 1, there was a significant test statistic of 15.36, p = 1.18e-6. Tukey's post hoc test indicated a significant difference between TBI and NAÏVE (p = 0.000004) and TBI and SHAM (p = 0.0001). On Day 2, there was a significant difference between groups (F = 9.49, p = 0.0002). Post-hoc testing revealed differences between TBI and NAÏVE (p = 0.0002) and TBI and SHAM (p = 0.005). On Day 3, the overall test statistic equals 6.27, p = 0.0025, indicating there are differences between the groups. Tukey's post-hoc test showed that again, TBI was different from NAÏVE and SHAM (p = 0.003, p = 0.035, respectively).

Using a one-way repeated measure ANOVA, differences between days within treatment groups were explored. First the assumption of sphericity was checked for each group. The within factor (Day) did not meet the sphericity assumption for the NAÏVE or SHAM groups, thus the continuity correction, Greenhouse-Grier was applied to those groups. For SHAM, there were no differences between days (p = 0.066), for NAÏVE and TBI there were (p = 0.006, p = 2.89E-7, respectively). Post-hoc comparisons showed for NAÏVE, the difference was between Day 0 and Day 1 (p = 0.003). For TBI, the differences were between Day 0 and Days 1, 2, and 3 (p = 9.2e-6, p = 0.0005, p = 0.002, respectively), and there was a difference between Day 1 and Day 3 (p = 0.018).

At 6 months after injury, there were no significant differences between NAÏVE, SHAM, or TBI on any day (Day 0, F = 0.315, p = 0.732; Day 1, F = 0.336, p = 0.717; Day 2, F = 0.5, p = 0.61; Day 3, F = 1.17, p = 0.322; Figure 4, right). When comparing differences between days within each group, there was a significant difference in the TBI group (p = 0.026), with Day 0 being different from Days 1, 2, and 3 (p = 0.026, p = 0.002, p = 0.002). There were no differences between any days for NAÏVE or SHAM (p =0.104, p = 0.063, respectively).

Data from the working memory version of the Morris water maze can be graphed in a variety of ways. Here we demonstrate the results for 3 months (Figure 5, left) and 12 months (Figure 5, right) after injury using both line graphs to represent the time course, and box plots to provide an overall summary of the data (Figure 5, bottom). We can then visualize Trial 1 comparisons and Trial 2 comparisons independently on each day as well as overall differences due to injury. Trial 1 latencies represent reference memory and Trial 2 latencies depict working memory.

The data from rats 3 months after injury are shown in Figure 5, left. For Trial 1 (Figure 5, upper left), when comparing NAÏVE, SHAM, and TBI, only Day 4 showed a significant difference between groups (F = 4.12, p = 0.025), with the post-hoc Tukey's test indicating that TBI was different from NAÏVE (p = 0.019). For Trial 2 (Figure 5, middle left), there was a significant difference on Day 1 (F = 5.93, p = 0.006), with post-hoc analysis indicating that TBI was different from SHAM (p = 0.005). The repeated measures ANOVA did not find an overall difference between injury groups at 3 months (p = 0.56). These results suggest that these rats have small yet significant deficits in reference as well as working memory at 3 months after injury.

At 12 months after injury, comparing Trial 1 NAÏVE, SHAM, and TBI (Figure 5, right), repeated measures ANOVA demonstrated a significant overall effect of injury (F = 3.94, p = 0.03). Pairwise comparisons revealed that TBI was significantly different from both NAÏVE and SHAM (p = 0.043 and p = 0.006., respectively) (Figure 5, bottom right). In addition, by comparing injury groups on each day, using a one-way ANOVA, a significant difference was detected on Day 3 (F = 7.28, p = 0.003). Post-hoc comparison revealed that TBI was different from SHAM (p = 0.0018) (Figure 5, upper right). For Trial 2, the repeated measures ANOVA found a significant difference due to injury (F = 3.97, p = 0.029), with post-hoc pairwise comparisons detecting the difference between TBI and SHAM (p = 0.017) (Figure 5, bottom right). One-way ANOVA on each day found significant differences on Days 2 and 4. On Day 2 (F = 4.02, p = 0.028), Tukey's post-hoc test found that TBI was different from SHAM (p = 0.023). On Day 4 (F = 4.12, p = 0.026), post-hoc analysis found a difference between TBI and SHAM (p = 0.025) (Figure 5, middle right).

Figure 1
Figure 1. Diagram of the water maze. This diagram demonstrates the possible platform locations (1, 2, 3, 4) and starting points (N, S, E, W) for the working memory Morris water maze. Rats are allowed two trials from each starting location/platform pairing. There is a 15 s inter-trial interval and 4 min rest in a warming chamber between pairs of trials for a total of four pairs of trials for each daily session. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Figure 2
Figure 2. Results of the neuroscore test. All rats were trained to simple reflex testing tasks prior to Day 0 (see text for details on training, testing, and scoring). Results are shown as median (black line), first and third quartiles (boundaries of box), and 10th and 90th percentiles (error bars). The mean is also shown by the red lines and outlying points as black dots. Data are presented for the Day 0 baseline and post-injury days 1-3. The results of the post-hoc t-test for each time point are shown on the graphs: * p < 0.001 vs TBI Day 0; ^ p < 0.001 vs same day NAÏVE. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Figure 3
Figure 3. Results of the Beam-Balance test. All rats were trained to balance on the beam until they could balance for 60 s for three consecutive trials (see text for details on training, testing and scoring). On subsequent tests, rats were scored on a scale from 1-6 with 1 signifying normal balance and 6 signifying no attempt to stay on the beam. Results are shown as median (black line), first and third quartiles (boundaries of box), and 10th and 90th percentiles (error bars). The mean is also shown by the red lines and outlying points as black dots. Data are presented for the Day 0 baseline score, post-injury days 1-3 (left), and 6 months after injury (right). The results of the post-hoc t-test for each time point are shown on the graphs. For Days 0-3: * p < 0.001vs TBI Day 0; ^ p < 0.001 vs same day NAÏVE; @ p < 0.001 vs same day SHAM. For 6 months: * p < 0.001vs TBI Day 0; # p < 0.001 vs NAÏVE Day 0; & p < 0.001 vs SHAM Day 0. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Figure 4
Figure 4. Results of the Beam-Walk test. All rats were trained to traverse the beam while weaving between posts to escape into a safety box. They were trained until they met criteria of ≤ 5 s on three consecutive trials (see text for details on training, testing and scoring). Baseline testing was completed on Day 0 and rats were subsequently tested on days 1-3 after injury (left). A subset of rats was also retested at 6 months after injury (right). The results are graphed as median (black line), first and third quartiles (boundaries of box), and 10th and 90th percentiles (error bars). The mean is also indicated by the red lines and outlying points as black dots. The outcomes of the post-hoc tests for each time point are shown on the graphs. For Days 0-3: * p < 0.001vs TBI Day 0; ^ p < 0.001 vs same day NAÏVE; @ p <0.001 vs same day SHAM; For 6 months: * p < 0.001vs TBI Day 0. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Figure 5
Figure 5. Results of the working memory Morris water maze. Results are shown for separate groups of rats at 3 months (left column) and 12 months (right column). The upper panels show the average latencies (time it took the rats to find the hidden platform) on the first trials of the two-trial pairing for each of the five testing days. The middle panels show the average latencies of the second trials on each day. Results of the post-hoc analysis are shown on the graphs (* p < 0.05 vs same day SHAM; ^ p < 0.05 vs same day NAÏVE). The lower panels summarize the results showing the median (black line), 25th and 75th percentiles (boundaries of box), and 10th and 90th percentiles (error bars). The mean is also shown by the red lines and outlying points as black dots. Results of the post-hoc analysis are shown on the graphs (*p < 0.05 vs same Trial SHAM, ^ p < 0.05 vs same Trial NAÏVE). Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Discussion

When conducting any type of behavioral testing, it is critical to be consistent. This detail includes many considerations that seem insignificant but have a major impact on the response of the animal. An important step that cannot be overlooked is acclimation of animals to their home-cage/housing situation prior to any experiment. This preparation reduces the effects of the animals' physiological stress response, which can alter behavioral outcomes18. Similarly, it is absolutely essential that every effort is made to handle all animals in the same manner. This consistency includes, as previously mentioned, acclimation to housing and also acclimation to handling and transportation between rooms prior to training or testing. This concept cannot be overstated. Sloppy animal handling is disastrous to any behavioral testing19. Likewise, every effort needs to be made to test animals at the same time of day, whether during their dark or light cycle. For the tests discussed here, testing during light or dark stage is acceptable, as long as the tests are performed consistently. Testing done at different times during the circadian cycle has been shown to alter behavioral outcomes18,20. Additionally, the handler as well as the animal must be in a stress free, calm state in order to maximize the accuracy of results.

Particularly in the case of the Neuroscore, false positives and negatives are common. False positives typically occur when an animal is not fully habituated to handling and testing. The animal must be completely relaxed so the observed response is reflexive and not due to muscles tightening from reacting out of stress or fear. A tense handler can influence the results by transmitting stress to the animal. Therefore, holding the rat too tight or too loose can both be problematic. Additionally, if the handler is nervous, this can confound the reaction of the rats. There is also the risk that an inexperienced observer will misinterpret the rat's response. Good training and a lot of practice are essential to the success and consistency of the Neuroscore.

In general, the chief concern with these tests is the lack of a large difference, and sometimes no difference, between treatment groups. Since animals can react differently to different handlers, noises, times of day, and potentially, seasons21, every effort must be made to reduce all possible confounding factors.

The results of the Beam-Balance and Beam-Walk tasks shown here demonstrate that these tests are useful early after injury to detect deficits in vestibulomotor function. These deficits typically resolve over time1,14. In this model, by 6 months after injury, the injury-induced deficits have resolved. The results of the 6 month time point indicate that there are no differences between NAIVE, SHAM, or injured rats; however, all the rats have been relaxing in their home cages for 6 months, aging and gaining weight. Thus, by the time they are re-tested at 6 months post-surgery (or equivalent in the case of NAÏVE), they are essentially becoming old and fat, and therefore all the groups do not perform as well as they did compared to their baseline Day 0 results.

Another important consideration is that the behavior test used is the correct test. For example, the tests employed here are thought to represent the function of specific brain areas. One example is the vestibular system, which is important for balance. Brain areas involved in sensorimotor function, such as the cortex including sensorimotor cortex, the thalamus, corticospinal neurons, basal ganglia, nigro-striatum, to name a few, are all involved in vestibulomotor coordination. Thus, deficits in the Beam-Balance or Beam-Walk indicate potential deficits in these areas. Furthermore, the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex are involved in the learning and memory functions tested by the working memory water maze. Even when the correct test is chosen, the limitations of the tests that are employed must be kept in mind. For example, none of the tests presented here are sensitive to deficits in mood, such as depression, anxiety, or social interactions such as aggression, decision making, or impulsivity. To reiterate, it is imperative to choose the appropriate test for the behavior and brain area to be evaluated.

Interpretation and analysis of behavioral data must be approached with caution. It is highly recommended to include power analyses of each type of test separately, because, using a behavioral outcome as a measure of a neural deficit, is by its nature, a crude measure of a subtle effect. Furthermore, different tests require different types of statistical analyses. For example, the Neuroscore and Beam-Balance tests described are dependent on the interpretation of a trained observer to score the behavior using an ordinal scale. These types of data are not continuous and not normally distributed, so nonparametric statistics should be used, such as the Kruskal-Wallis test, as demonstrated in Sections 6.1 and 6.2. Alternatively, the Beam-Walk and working memory water maze tests produce data that are continuous and normally distributed, so parametric statistics can be used, such as one-way ANOVA or repeated-measures two-way ANOVA, as demonstrated in Sections 6.3 and 6.4.

The behavioral tasks presented here have stood the test of time and give reproducible results, particularly when paired with the FPI model in rats, although many other methods of behavioral testing for brain injury do exist. The neuroscore is a short assessment performed with a minimum of equipment. Other tests of reflexes and strength are available and could be incorporated into a neurological assessment, such as the lateral pulsion task, the akinesia test, the inclined plane test, and grip strength (see Fujimoto et al.22 and Gold et al.23). The Beam-Balance and Beam-Walk tasks described are measures of vestibulomotor deficits after injury. Vestibulomotor coordination can be considered one measure of gross locomotor behavior, while other measures of gross locomotor deficits include the Rotarod, the rotating pole, and open field activity. The ability to swim, measured as swim speed during the water maze, is also an indication of gross motor coordination22,23. The working memory water maze task completes this set of tests by detecting both reference memory deficits (indicated by Trial 1) and working memory deficits (indicated by Trial 2 or the difference between Trial 1 and Trial 2). Other measures of cognitive function include the eight arm radial maze, the Barnes maze, the novel object recognition test, and different variations of the water maze. These variations include the original Morris water maze and the Lashley III maze (again see Fujimoto et al.22 and Gold et al.23). This battery of tests has proven to be useful early after injury and, in varying degrees, out to 12 months after injury1.

Additionally, the tasks demonstrated here can be used with different strains, sex, and ages of rats; however, accommodations may need to be made for differing sizes and in cases of greater frailty. For example, older, heavier rats need a wider beam for the Beam-Balance task and aged, frail rats, may need shorter duration of swim times in the water maze. Thus, there is room for flexibility in these tests and potential for development of new tests to accommodate different situations and hypotheses.

Disclosures

The authors have nothing to disclose.

Acknowledgments

We thank Ian Bolding for assistance with surgical preparation of subjects and Elizabeth Sumner for her meticulous editing. These studies were completed as part of a team funded by The Moody Project for Translational Traumatic Brain Injury Research.

Materials

Name Company Catalog Number Comments
Sprague-Dawley rats Charles Rivers Laboratories
251 Ballardvale St
Wilmington, MA 01887-1096
Phone: 800-522-7287
CD-IGS rats, strain code 001 male, albino, 300-350g at arrival
Name Company Catalog Number Comments
Beam-Balance
Beam home built wood, 25" l x 1" h x 3/4" w sealed with polyurethane varnish
C-clamp Home Depot 1422-C 2 1/2"
barrier Home Depot styrofoam, 18" x 17 1/2"
table (for both BB & BW) generic office supply 37" h x 30" w x 60" l
Name Company Catalog Number Comments
Beam-Walk
Beam home built wood 38-1/2" l x 1-3/4" h x 1" w sealed with polyurethane varnish (~ 37" off floor)
escape box home built woodpainted black 12 1/2 " l x 9" h x 7-1/4" w
nails (pegs) 2"
hinges
clamps
white noise machine San Diego Instruments
9155 Brown Deer Rd, Suite 8 San Diego, CA 92121
Phone: (858)530-2600
http://www.sandiegoinstruments.com/libraries/misc/datasheets/whitenoise.pdf
light Home Depot
Name Company Catalog Number Comments
Morris Water Maze
fiberglass pool manufacturer unknown
(similar to one made by SDI) San Diego Instruments 7000-0723 72" diameter x 30" deep (~ 500 gal)
plexiglass platform hand-made by Maggie Parsley 10 cm diameter, 26" tall with silicone applied to the surface of the platform to provide a gripping surface
(similar to one made by SDI) SDI 7500-0272
plexiglass animal boxes w/ lids UTMB Machine Shop 2 boxes, 10" w x 16" L x 9" h
spot lights/ heat lamps Home Depot 3 around pool, 2 over boxes to dry animals
AnyMaze San Diego Instruments
9155 Brown Deer Rd, Suite 8 San Diego, CA 92121
Phone: (858)530-2600
/9001 http://www.sandiegoinstruments.com/any-maze-video-tracking/

DOWNLOAD MATERIALS LIST

References

  1. Sell, S. L., Johnson, K., DeWitt, D. S., Prough, D. S. Persistent Behavioral Deficits in Rats after Fluid Percussion Injury. J Neurotrauma. 34 (5), 1086-1096 (2017).
  2. Gurdjian, E. S., Lissner, H. R., Webster, J. E., Latimer, F. R., Haddad, B. F. Studies on experimental concussion: relation of physiologic effect to time duration of intracranial pressure increase at impact. Neurology. 4, 674-681 (1954).
  3. Anonymous. US National Library of Medicine. , (last accessed Jan. 9, 2018) www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/$term=fluid+percussion+injury+rat (2017).
  4. Hellmich, H. L., Capra, B., Eidson, K., Garcia, J., Kennedy, D., Uchida, T., et al. Dose-dependent neuronal injury after traumatic brain injury. Brain Research. 1044, 144-154 (2005).
  5. Bramlett, H. M., Detrich, W. D. Long-Term consequences of Traumatic Brain Injury: Current Status of Potential Mechanisms of Injury and Neurological Outcomes. J. Neurotrauma. 32, 1-15 (2015).
  6. Pierce, J. E. S., Smith, D. H., Trojanowski, J. Q., McIntosh, T. K. Enduring cognitive neurobehavioral and histopathological changes persist for up to one year following severe experimental brain injury in rats. Neuroscience. 87 (2), 359-369 (1998).
  7. Smith, D. H., Chen, X. H., Pierce, J. E. S., Wolf, J. A., Trojanowski, J. Q., Graham, D. I., McIntosh, T. K. Progressive atrophy and neuron death for one year following brain trauma in the rat. J Neurotrauma. 14 (10), 715-727 (1997).
  8. Schallert, T. Behavioral tests for preclinical intervention assessment. NeuroRx. 3 (4), 497-504 (2006).
  9. Sell, S. L., Avila, M. A., Yu, G., Vergara, L., Prough, D. S., Grady, J. J., DeWitt, D. S. Hypertonic resuscitation improves neuronal and behavioral outcomes after traumatic brain injury plus hemorrhage. Anesthesiology. 108 (5), 873-881 (2008).
  10. Hamm, R. J. Neurobehavioral assessment of outcome following traumatic brain injury in rats: an evaluation of selected measures. J Neurotrauma. 18 (11), 1207-1216 (2001).
  11. Feeney, D. M., Gonzalez, A., Law, W. A. Amphetamine, haloperidol, and experience interact to affect rate of recovery after motor cortex injury. Science. 217, 855-857 (1982).
  12. Hamm, R. J., Temple, M. D., Pike, B. R., O'Dell, D. M., Buck, D. L., Lyeth, B. G. Working memory deficits following traumatic brain injury in the rat. J Neurotrauma. 13, 317-323 (1996).
  13. Morris, R. G., Hagan, J. J. Allocentric spatial learning by hippocampectomised rats: A further test of the "Spatial Mapping" and "Working Memory" Theories of hippocampal function. The Quart J of Exp Psych. 38 (4), 365-395 (1986).
  14. Dixon, C. E., Lyeth, B. G., Povlishock, J. T., Findling, R. L., Hamm, R. J., Marmarou, A., Young, H. F., Hayes, R. L. A fluid-percussion model of experimental brain injury in the rat. J. Neurosurg. 67, 110-119 (1987).
  15. DeWitt, D. S., Smith, T. G., Deyo, D. J., Miller, K. R., Uchida, T., Prough, D. S. L-arginine and superoxide dismutase prevent or reverse cerebral hypoperfusion after fluid-percussion traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma. 14, 223-233 (1997).
  16. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. , R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria. (2017).
  17. Pohlert, T. The Pairwise Multiple comparison of Mean Ranks Package (PMCMR) R package. , http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=PMCMR (2014).
  18. Verma, P., Hellemans, K. G., Choi, F. Y., Yu, W., Weinberg, J. Circadian phase and sex effects on depressive/anxiety-like behaviors and HPA axis responses to acute stress. Physiol Behav. 99, 276-285 (2010).
  19. Schallert, T., Woodlee, M. T., Fleming, S. M. Experimental Focal Ischemic Injury: Behavior-Brain Interactions and Issues of Animal Handling and Housing. ILAR J. 44 (2), 130-143 (2003).
  20. Ruis, J. F., Rietveld, W. J., Buys, J. P. Properties of parametric photic entrainment of circadian rhythms in the rat. Physiol Behav. 50, 1233-1239 (1991).
  21. Ferguson, S. A., Maier, K. L. A review of seasonal/circannual effects of laboratory rodent behavior. Physiol Behav. 119, 130-136 (2003).
  22. Fujimoto, S. T., Longhi, L., Saatman, K. E., McIntosh, T. K. Motor and cognitive function evaluation following experimental traumatic brain injury. Neurosci & Biobehav Reviews. , (2004).
  23. Gold, E. M., Su, D., Lopez-Velazquez, L., Haus, D. L., Perez, H., Lacuesta, G. A., Anderson, A. J., Cummings, B. J. Functional assessment of long-term deficits in rodent models of traumatic brain injury. Regen. Med. 8 (4), 483-516 (2013).

Tags

Behavioral Deficits Rats Traumatic Brain Injury Neurological Function Cognitive Function Brain Injury Research Experimental Treatment Blast Injury Stroke Reproducible Techniques Rat Handling Experience Acclimation Forelimb Flexion Test Hindlimb Flexion Test
Detecting Behavioral Deficits in Rats After Traumatic Brain Injury
Play Video
PDF DOI DOWNLOAD MATERIALS LIST

Cite this Article

Hausser, N., Johnson, K., Parsley,More

Hausser, N., Johnson, K., Parsley, M. A., Guptarak, J., Spratt, H., Sell, S. L. Detecting Behavioral Deficits in Rats After Traumatic Brain Injury. J. Vis. Exp. (131), e56044, doi:10.3791/56044 (2018).

Less
Copy Citation Download Citation Reprints and Permissions
View Video

Get cutting-edge science videos from JoVE sent straight to your inbox every month.

Waiting X
Simple Hit Counter